Cats meow at humans more than at their fellow felines. The reasons why are complicated.
(Image credit: Africa Studio via Shutterstock)
This is a story that goes back thousands of years.
Originally, cats were solitary creatures. This means they preferred to live and hunt alone, rather than in groups. Most of their social behaviour was restricted to mother-kitten interactions. Outside of this relationship, cats rarely meow at each other.
However, as cats began to live alongside humans, these vocalisations took on new meanings. In many ways, when a cat meows at us, it’s as if they see us as their caregivers, much like their feline mothers.
Cats probably first encountered humans roughly 10,000 years ago, when people began establishing permanent settlements. These settlements attracted rodents, which in turn drew cats looking for prey. The less fearful and more adaptable cats thrived, benefiting from a consistent food supply. Over time, these cats developed closer bonds with humans.
RELATED: Cats have nearly 300 facial expressions, including a ‘play face’ they share with humans
Unlike dogs, which were bred by humans for specific traits, cats essentially domesticated themselves. Those that could tolerate and communicate with humans had a survival advantage, leading to a population well-suited to living alongside people.
To understand this process, we can look at Russian farmed fox experiments. Beginning in the 1950s, Soviet scientist Dmitry Belyaev and his team selectively bred silver foxes, mating those that were less fearful and aggressive toward humans.
Get the world’s most fascinating discoveries delivered straight to your inbox.
Over generations, these foxes became more docile and friendly, developing physical traits similar to domesticated dogs, such as floppy ears and curly tails. Their vocalisations changed too, shifting from aggressive “coughs” and “snorts” to more friendly “cackles” and “pants”, reminiscent of human laughter.
These experiments demonstrated that selective breeding for tameness could lead to a range of behavioural and physical changes in animals, achieving in a few decades what would usually take thousands of years. Though less obvious than the differences between dogs and the ancestral wolf, cats have also changed since their days as African wildcats. They now have smaller brains and more varied coat colours, traits common among many domestic species.
Cats’ vocal adaptations
Like the silver foxes, cats have adapted their vocalisations, albeit over a much longer period of time. Human babies are altrical at birth, meaning they are entirely dependent on their parents. This dependency has made us particularly attuned to distress calls — ignoring them would be costly for human survival.
This owner looks like he would struggle to ignore his cat’s distress call. (Image credit: Magui RF via Shutterstock)
Cats have altered their vocalisations to tap into this sensitivity. A 2009 study by animal behaviour researcher Karen McComb and her team gives evidence of this adaptation. Participants in the study listened to two types of purrs. One type was recorded when cats were seeking food (solicitation purr) and another recorded when they were not (non-solicitation purr). Both cat owners and non-cat owners rated the solicitation purrs as more urgent and less pleasant.
An acoustic analysis revealed a high-pitch component in these solicitation purrs, resembling a cry. This hidden cry taps into our innate sensitivity to distress sounds, making it nearly impossible for us to ignore.
But it’s not just cats that have adapted their vocalisations: we have too. When we talk to babies, we use “motherese”, more commonly known as “baby talk”, characterised by a higher pitch, exaggerated tones and simplified language. This form of speech helps engage infants, playing a role in their language development.
We’ve extended this style of communication to our interactions with pets, known as pet-directed speech. Recent research suggests that cats respond to this form of communication. A 2022 study by animal behaviour researcher Charlotte de Mouzon and colleagues found that cats could distinguish between speech addressed to them and speech addressed to adult humans. This pattern of discrimination was particularly strong when the speech came from the cats’ owners.
Our adoption of pet-directed speech reinforces a bond that mirrors mother-kitten interactions.
Changes in vocalisations are not only seen in cat-human relationships. Compared to the ancestral wolf, dogs have expanded their barking behaviour to communicate more effectively with humans and, just as with cats, we use pet-directed speech when interacting with dogs.
Over time, cats have evolved to use vocal signals that resonate with our nurturing instincts. Paired with our use of pet-directed speech, this two-way communication highlights the unique relationship we’ve developed with our feline friends. It seems cats might be the winners in this relationship, adapting to solicit care and attention from us. Still, a lot of cat owners wouldn’t have it any other way.
This edited article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.
Grace is a Lecturer in Animal Welfare and Behaviour at the School of Psychology, Queen’s University Belfast. She holds a First Class Honours BSc in Applied Psychology (2011) and an MSc in Animal Behaviour and Welfare, with Distinction (2012). In 2017, she completed her PhD in Farm Animal Welfare Science.
Her research focuses on farm animal welfare, particularly pigs, focusing on harmful behaviours, and post-mortem indicators of welfare. She also investigates cat welfare, the psychology of pet-keeping, and strategies for changing human behaviour to improve animal welfare. Grace’s work aims to enhance the well-being of animals and promote better human-animal relationships.
>>> Read full article>>>
Copyright for syndicated content belongs to the linked Source : Live Science – https://www.livescience.com/animals/cats-love-to-meow-at-humans-now-we-know-why